Friday, August 31, 2007
Example Concept Map: Using MS Word and MW Snap
See Creating a concept map for the steps I followed. Note that I used MW Snap rather than MS Paint because it resulted in a higher resolution.
Figure 1. An example concept map in Blogger, created using MS Word and MW Snap (Hart, 2007).
I like the flow-chart nature of the concept map, but I think the dark shading makes the text difficult to read.
Wednesday, August 29, 2007
How to embed a MindMeister concept map
- Create a MindMeister account
- Create a MindMeister concept map
- Click on Share (at the bottom of a map)
- Click on Publish
- Check make available on the internet
- Copy the embed code
- Save
- Create a new blog post
- Click on Html
- Paste the embed code
- Save
- I suggest you put an embedded MindMeister concept map its own posting. Then link to it from your main blog essay posting. There seem to be some problems with other text not being viewable in the Compose editing when embedded MindMeister code is used.
- Also be aware that MindMeister disables the ability to embed after one month on the free account. The map is still there and can be exported, etc., but it can't be embedded into other pages unless you upgrade (or open up a new account).
- Provide some brief instructions to the reader on how to read/use the map, e.g., (You can click, drag, and zoom)
Kid Nation: 21st century Lord of the Flies?
CBS' reality TV show Kid Nation, due for release mid-September, 2007 has 40 kids aged 8 to 15 living together for 40 days in an abandoned New Mexico town, with no adults. The program offers a modern day version of William Golding's "Lord of the Flies". Can children can create an effective society on their own?
From a social psychological point of view, the program stands to offer a fascinating examination and example of theories such as our social selves, prejudice, group processes, leadership, communication, and so on.
From an experiential education point of view, it will be interesting to see how well the children were able to adapt and learn without direct adult guidance.
Currently, however, the show is attracting considerable media and legal attention, with claims of violating children's human rights. See this current google news search, with article headlines such as:
- CBS should pull plug on "Kid" reality show
- Leave the kids alone
- Show accused of exploiting children
- Critics slam new kids reality TV show
- Elements of 'Kid Nation' detestable
- Did CBS Neglect the Kid Nation Kids?
For more info, also see Kid Nation on Wikipedia.
Monday, August 27, 2007
Marketing: Ready, Set, Go, Bruce!
Wednesday, August 22, 2007
Student blog examples from drop-in tutorial
- Ladyzulu worked out how to:
- Copy an essay draft from MS Word into a blog posting - the formatting came through quite nicely - feel free to read and comment (she's the 1st person to publish a blog draft - congratulations! :) ).
- Create a concept map in MS Word, using the Drawing toolbar, then using PrintScreen and MS Paint to capture and save the image, and then uploading the image into a blog posting - see example concept map. For more info on how to do this, see creating a concept map.
- Amanda worked out how to:
- Copy a table from an MS Word document into a blog posting - the formatting came through very well - see example.
- Embed a video from youtube in a blog posting - see example.
- Add a photo to one's blogger profile
- Fiona was keen to embrace the possibilities of online concept mapping using MindMeister. She had already created a concept map for her essay and we then worked out how to embed the map into a blog posting - see example. There are several really appealing features of this approach, e.g.,
- a detailed, readable map can be presented
- zoom and scroll
- nodes can be hyperlinked
- collaborative maps can be developed
- free, online
Thursday, August 16, 2007
Lecture 4: Reactions to "Ghosts of Rwanda"
So, my hat's off to those who have already started blogging their reactions.
I suspect we experience our own mini-psychological trauma when exposed to this kind of story and footage about human brutality on a mass social scale. I go through a kind of mini-depression for a few days after engaging in details of what happened. But I also believe that by going into some of the most difficult topics in psychology, we can emerge with a richer understanding and stronger capability for achieving more desirable outcomes.
However, I don't think we should move too lightly over our emotional reactions. They need processing - this might be a private or shared process. And then gradually as we emerge, we can explore how the events can be understood at least from a social psychological perspective.
There is more background information on the film's website (Ghosts of Rwanda), including the full transcript (thanks kelg85). There is also a lot of documentation from various sources and perspectives about the Rwandan genocide the broader societal conflict and issues (e.g.,The Rwandan Conflict: Origin, Development, Exit Strategies). Not much of this material is from social psychological perspective per se, and little seems to be by Rwandans themselves (perhaps an indication of the cultural shock and decimation). Nevertheless, books such as the following are recommended if you want to delve further:
- Dallaire, R., & Power, S. (2004). Shake hands with the devil: The failure of humanity in Rwanda. Carrol & Graf.
- Gourevitch, P. (1999). We wish to inform you that tomorrow we will be killed with our families: Stories from Rwanda. Picador.
- Straus, S. (2006). The order of genocide: Race, power, and war in Rwanda. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.
- For more suggestions, see
- Rwandan 1994 Genocide (UCSpace)
- Bibliography of the Rwandan Genocide (Wikipedia)
- Gacaca Courts (based on principles of restorative justice)
- Ingando Solidarity Camps
- Evaluation of the National Unity and Reconciliation Commission (NURC)
- Outward Bound Rwanda
- Looking for more suggestions!
Thursday, August 9, 2007
The Survival of Psychiatric Diagnosis - Prof. David Pilgrim
Personally, I've always been kind of puzzled by psychiatric and clinical psychological committment to categorisation. Perhaps this was revealed by the audience questions which seemed to struggle with Pilgrim's suggestion that we might try approaching clinical psychological work without such heavy reliance on the social constructs of psychiatric diagnosis.
With our recent talk in the social psychology unit about schemas, I could more readily see how schemas apply to, and explain both the convenience and potential consequences of psychiatric categorisation. Diagnosis seems to offer the illusion of cognitive control over the behaviour of others. Kingsley Tonkin (one of the new clinical psych. staff at UC) made the point, for example, that diagnosis was being used as a way of acting on fear and uncertainty about indigenous people's potential future actions in Queensland.
It is easy to talk about this from a philosophical view, but I thought it was great that in the audience were individuals with so-called psychiatric issues who responded to Pilgrim's provocative views. This gave the session an extra, challenging, realism.
As an undergraduate psychology student, I remember writing an article for the university newspaper about the rapid expansion in the diagnostic categories for mental illness which have occurred in the last 100 or so years. In comparison, our vocabulary and understanding about psychological well-being had progressed little. I suggested that it might be time invest more energy in exploring and mapping out the realms of psychological well-being such as, for example, the work Ken Wilber has been doing.
Interestingly, we don't tend to diagnose and categorise well-being, but do tend to with distress. We tend to allow for diverse forms of psychological well-being without imposing artificial labels but when threatened by bouts of so-called madness, Western psychological and psychiatric culture seems to default to labeling in order to achieve a sense of control and protection.
This is not to say Western psychology hasn't achieved much and many parts of it have clearly helped many people towards improved psychological well-being and effectiveness. But many seem to forget that psychology is a young science, with much of its psychological practice heavily embedded in political and cultural agendas. So, I found it refreshing to hear David Pilgrim's warning about the potentially blinkering, limiting, and even inhumane effects of our psychiatric and clinical psychological industries and professions currently being dominated in their weltanschauung by a culturally constructed set of debatable symptom categories.
-------
Professor David Pilgrim
Professor of Mental Health Policy, University of Central Lancashire UK and Consultant Clinical Psychologist Lancashire Care NHS Trust.
Thursday 9 August 4:30pm
"The survival of Psychiatric Diagnosis"
Professor Pilgrim will examine past and current debates about applying medical diagnoses to psychological difference in society, beginning with a brief historical overview from antiquity to 'anti-psychiatry' and a summary of recent debates. The paper then offers two case studies of common diagnoses ('depression' and 'schizophrenia').
Professor Pilgrim believes the main challenge for social science is no longer about what is wrong with psychiatric diagnosis (that is now well rehearsed) but accounting for how and why it has survived. In answering this question about survival, inter-disciplinary work could attend to the pre-empirical positions of mental health researchers; the ways in which mental disorders are similar and different to physical disorders; and the interest in work of different social groups defending or attacking psychiatric diagnoses in varying contexts.
When: Thursday 9 August, 4:30-6pm
Where: University of Canberra, 12B50
David Pilgrim is Professor of Mental Health Policy, University of Central Lancashire and Consultant Clinical Psychologist Lancashire Care NHS Trust. His work over the past 20 years has been split between the NHS and higher education. Hispublications include: Pilgrim, D. (2005) Key Concepts in Mental Health, London: Sage; Cheshire, K. and Pilgrim, D. (2004) Clinical Psychology: A Short Introduction, London: Sage; Rogers, A. and Pilgrim, D. (2003) Mental Health and Inequality, Basingstoke: Palgrave; Rogers, A. and Pilgrim, D. (2001) Mental Health Policy in Britain (2nd Edition), Basingstoke: Palgrave. The third edition of A Sociology of Mental Health and Illness (with A. Rogers for Open University Press) won the BMA book award for 2006.
Also see comments on this presentation by:
Sunday, August 5, 2007
Bandura, Social Learning, Aggression, & Bobo dolls (Resources)
- A four-minute video of Bandura discussing his research (Google Video). Clips from the Bobo doll study appear about 2 minutes and 40 seconds.
- Bobo doll experiment (Wikipedia)
- The full text of the seminal Bandura, Ross, and Ross article is available from “Classics in the History of Psychology”: Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. (1961). Imitation of film-mediated aggressive models. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66, 3-11.
Wednesday, August 1, 2007
Lecture 3: Social Thinking (Reflections)
I suggested that these cognitive mechanisms provided the underlying architecture for the occurrence of more observable social phenomenon, including extreme events such as genocide, which can be seen as "perfect storms" of social psychological variables coming together within particularly cultural contexts.
In this lecture, we consider the role of our cognitive mechanisms in the formation and changing of attitudes, and their application in social influence and persuasion.
Key Concepts
- Social Cognition
- Cognitive Miser (and the duplex mind)
- Knowledge structures
- Schemata
- Scripts
- Stereotypes
- Priming
- Framing
- Attributions
- Internal vs. external
- Correspondence Bias (or Fundamental Attribution Error)
- Actor-observer bias
- Self-serving bias
- Heuristics
- Social Perception
- Attitudes
- Mere-exposure effect
- Cognitive dissonance
- Influence and persuasion
- Compliance and conformity
- Normative
- Informational
- Minority
Humans are cognitive misers in that we use our conscious mind relatively little, with most of the processing being done by the automatic mind (also see duplex mind). The automatic mind uses a variety of adaptive, cognitive shortcuts to reduce cognitive load. These schemas, scripts, stereotypes, heuristics, and so on, all serve to efficiently process complex environmental and social information, and help to provide a sense of control and predictability over our social environment.
For observed events and behaviours which are common (i.e., fit norms, schemata and scripts), our automatic mind does most of the processing. For unusual events and behaviours humans tend to want to find explanation (attribution).
There appears to be a natural tendency to connect observed behaviour with an actor's disposition. This is called correspondent inference. When we infer an actor's disposition from a behaviour and its consequences and play down possible situational explanations we demonstrate what is know as the correspondence bias or the fundamental attribution error (FAE).
We are more likely to make a FAE when dealing with others than with ourselves. We give more credence to situational explanations for our own behaviour than we do when interpreting the behaviour of others. This is the actor-observer effect.
We further demonstrate self-serving bias by tending to attribute our successes to dispositional causes and our failures to external causes; whereas we tend to do the opposite when explaining other people's behaviour.
The ultimate attribution error occurs when we use these biases in the context of groups, e.g., we tend to make self-serving attributions about our in-groups (the groups to which we belong and identify with) and out-groups.
Heuristics, schemata, scripts, stereotypes, etc. can be usefully thought of as algorithmic "nets" or "filters" which we use to catch certain kinds of information and which help us to navigate typically encountered social environments. The occurrence of unusual events is more likely to flag conscious awareness which (if cognitive capacity is available) and can then trigger consciousness thinking and consideration. However, we tend to prefer and attend more to information which is consistent with our expectations, making it difficult to change existing, largely automatic cognitive processing of our social environment.
Whilst the extent to which these cognitive biases occur is somewhat innate, it also varies across cultures. For example, the FAE is fair more prevalent in the US than in India.
Attitudes
Attitudes were the first major area of study in cognitively focused social psychology.
Attitudes are valenced dispositions we experience towards people, objects, ideas, and events. Attitudes are the basic experience of like (attraction) or dislike (revulsion) which we experience in varying strengths to just about everything.
Attitudes tend to become polarized (become more exaggerated) over time.
Attitudes can manifest in thinking (cognition), feeling (emotion), and behaviour.
Attitudes are acquired via:
- Mere-exposure effect
- Classical conditioning
- Operant conditioning
- Social learning
Attitude research has been criticised for the lack of relationship to actual behaviour. Attitudes are most likely to predict behaviour:
- when the attitude is specifically related to a specific behaviour
- over time and situations (as opposed to behaviour in a specific situation at a specific time)
- when the behaviour is freely chosen
- Changing attitude
- Changing behaviour
- Using defence mechanisms to deal with the dissonance (i.e., push awareness of the dissonance out of the conscious mind)
Influence and Persuasion
Influence refers to ways in which an individual's attitudes and behaviour is altered by their perceptions of the social environment.
Normative influence occurs when your behaviour conforms with the majority, i.e., you do what everyone else does (e.g., Asch's Conformity Experiment). Going along with the majority, however, doesn't mean that your private attitude is consistent with the majority. It just means that your external behaviour fits with normative behaviour.
Informational influence occurs particularly in situations which are ambiguous or you are under cognitive load or you lack expertise. In such situations not only is your behaviour likely to conform with the majority, you are also likely to believe that other people are correct.
Influence techniques consist of various ways in which people can be manipulated into changing attitudes/behaviours, e.g., foot-in-the-door (start with a small request, then build up).
Minority influence occurs when even a single individual is a dissident (i.e., goes against the norm). Particularly when a minority is consistent in its alternative message, it can have a disproportionate influence on swaying the majority.
Persuasion is the intentional use of psychological influence techniques in order to get a person or group to adopt a desired behaviour (e.g., purchase a produce) or change an attitude (e.g., become pro-abortion).
The successfulness of an attempt at persuasion can be seen as relying on:
- From Whom (credibility, likability)
- Message Content (reason, emotion)
- Audience
- Alpha strategies (increasing the strong, compelling, credible argument)
- Omega strategies (reducing/removing barriers/resistance)